Thursday, March 28, 2024
HomeFor ProfessionalsBringing Neuroscience Into the Therapy Session

Bringing Neuroscience Into the Therapy Session

Bringing Neuroscience Into the Therapy Session

By Ralph Carson, PhDRalph

The anorectic’s brain is a complicated structure that is influenced by genetics and the environment. This duality epitomizes the loaded-gun theory, in that who our parents are (genetics) provides the ammunition and how we live our life (starvation) pulls the trigger. Numerous adolescents go on very-low-calorie diets every year, but less than 1 percent continue to progress on to anorexia allowing them to maintain an extremely low weight for years (Bulik, Slof-Op’t Landt, van Furth, & Sullivan, 2007; Hudson et al., 2007). The genetic vulnerability may remain latent for the first decade of life until it is precipitated by circumstances (puberty, social media, peer pressure) and perpetuated by severe food restriction, starvation, and weight loss. Anorexia can be considered a biopsychosocial condition whereby a physiological change is brought on by an emotional response.

Fifty to seventy percent of the risk of developing an eating disorder is genetic (Bulik et al., 2007; Cui et al., 2013). Though identifying a single gene would simplify early identification, the fact remains that multiple genes interact to produce a wide spectrum of symptoms and behaviors. A genetic mouse model (BDNF val66met) has been useful in conducting research to elucidate further understanding of the disorder that may contribute to treatment efficacy (Madra & Zeltser, 2016). The genes themselves are activated to produce proteins that are encoded into neural processes. Starvation turns on genetic transcription factors that initiate the course of events leading to anorectic behaviors. Pre-existing genetic traits define the individual’s temperaments, character qualities, and personalities that profile the anorectic and persist after recovery (Cloninger et al., 1993, 1994).

Starvation, malnutrition, and weight restoration must be addressed during the acute stages of recovery. Depression, irritability, food obsessions, rituals, difficulty concentrating, mental fatigue, memory decline, apathy, and hopelessness are manifestations of starvation (van der Zee, 1998; Keys et al., 1950). When the body is appropriately nourished and weight is restored to 90 percent of ideal, many of the psychiatric symptoms can be reversed. Lack of food and immense stress can cause cellular loss, but, fortunately, the brain is malleable and the damage can be reversed with appropriate dietary intervention. Many in the treatment field argue that the order or priority in the treatment of anorexia should be: “First we eat, then we talk.” Thought processes and commitment to change are not responsive to therapy until the brain is fully engaged, which entails replacing lost brain tissue (Zakzanis et al., 2010; Lauer et al., 1999). Starvation is a state condition, and the disturbances attributed to that state are normalized after weight restoration (Marzola et al., 2013).

Constructing a visual picture of healing and recovery instills confidence in the treatment and, thereby, increases the likelihood of successful outcomes. Brain scans from those struggling with anorexia show brain cell loss similar to that seen with dementia (Alzheimer’s). Observing the return of gray matter volume can provide encouragement that one can get all of his or her brain substance and function back (Zakzanis et al., 2010; Lauer et al., 1999).

It is beyond the scope of this paper to explain neuroscientifically the multitude of symptomology, behaviors, and traits associated with anorexia nervosa. Following is a brief summary to explain the various differences (as opposed to calling them deficits) through the language of neuroscience (Halmi et al., 1991, 2000, 2004; Kaye et al., 1996, 1998, 1999a, b, c, 2001, 2002, 2003, 2005, 2008a, b, 2009, 2011; Lask et al., 2005, 2011; Cloninger, 1986; Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993; Fassino et al., 2002; Liu, 1979, 2007; Klump et al., 2004; Roberts & Pomerantz, 2004; Roberts et al., 2007, 2010; Tchanturia et al., 2007; Lopez et al., 2004):

  • Anhedonia: Increased binding by D2D3 receptors in the anterior ventral striatum
  • Ascetism: Increased binding by D2D3 receptors in the dorsal striatum
  • Impaired set shifting and persistence: Increased thalamic D2 binding and alterations in the rostral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex
  • Perfectionism: Over-activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and hyperfunction of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex
  • Harm avoidance and anxiety: Enhanced 5-HT1A binding in the amygdala and hypothalamus; reduced 5-HT2A binding in the amygdala and cortex
  • Impulse control and crossover to bulimia nervosa: Interconversion of the long and short alleles of the 5-HTTLPR (serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region)
  • Reduced appetite (anorexia): Reduced interoceptive response (anterior insula) with diminished awareness and an unresponsive ventral striatum
  • Body image impairment: Insular dysfunction; allocentric lock and an inability to update body image
  • Problems with central coherence: Visuospatial difficulties stemming from increased parietal activity
  • Compulsive exercising (foraging and hoarding behavior): Unmasking NPY2
  • Hyperactivity (excessive exercise) and rituals: Serotonin deficits
  • Low reward dependence: Norepinephrine dysregulation

Anorexic patients also can struggle with the following:

carson

Recognizing the severe consequences that are associated with anorexia, one questions how a genetic predisposition to anorexia would survive in the gene pool over time to the present. The adaptive flee hypothesis attempts to explain this condition as an evolutionary instinct for helping primitive man cope with famine (Guisinger, 2003; Kersting, 2004). In Paleolithic times, there were numerous survival threats that influenced the availability of food (climatic conditions, including droughts and freezing winters, as well as pestilence and predators). The ensuing starvation brought on extreme hunger, weakness, and fatigue that prohibited the searching and energy necessary to secure a source of food. However, these stark conditions activated genes in a select few that were unfazed by starvation and capable to forage for food when others were incapacitated. These unique individuals were not only equipped with the energy to find food, but also demonstrated remarkable restraint to refuse consumption in order to feed others. Once food became plentiful, the family or clan would refeed the foragers enough to reproduce and pass on their genes to future generations.

This strategy for times of hardship when famines were imminent was duplicated during the Dutch starvation winter of 1945 (van der Zee, 1998). Nearing the end of World War II, the Hitler regime punished the Dutch for their siding with the Allies by providing them with limited food rations. It was reported that individuals traveled considerable distances on foot or on bikes at great physical expense to forage for food. These same volunteers refused to eat what they found, despite starving themselves. This behavior reflects a form of hoarding, but also characteristic behavior of those with anorexia (Ammar et al., 2000; Bair, 2006; Bartness, Keen-Rhinehart, Dailey, & Teubner, 2011; Beaumont, 1996; Cabanac & Swiergiel, 1989).

Researchers have identified an increase level of NPY1 (neuropeptide Y) in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus (feeding center of the brain) that normally turns on the drive for food. However, during times of famine, NPY2 is unasked which contrarily inhibits the consumption of food and directs attention only to food acquisition (Ammar et al., 2000; Baird, Gray, & Fischer, 2006; Bartness et al., 2011; Beaumont, 1996; Cabanac & Swiergiel, 1989). This may be an explanation as to what occurs when starvation turns on the gene in anorectics during times of extreme food restriction.

It would be natural to conclude that recovery is at the mercy of our genes over which we have no control because we can’t choose our parents. Fortunately, heredity is not destiny (Kagan & Snidman, 2004; Kagan, 2010; LeDoux, 1996, 2001). Traits are not a pure category in that they differ in degree and fall on a continuum. They are only one dimension of our personality. Genes do not directly affect our traits, but they direct cells to synthesize proteins that shape our nervous systems (Cozolino, 2010; Tryon, 2014; Dharani, 2014). Our brains are forgiving, and cognitive flexibility is possible through therapy and medical intervention (Saxena & Feursner, 2006; Simpson & Campbell, 2013). The instruments of change at the disposal of the healing brain are epigenetics, resilience, collateral repair, neuroregeneration, and neuroplasticity. Each of these neurological mechanisms is activated by various therapeutic interventions. Some of the more familiar therapeutic interventions include cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), dialectical behavioral therapy (DBT), acceptance and commitment therapy, and exposure response prevention. Additional tools include medication, surgery, nutrition, activity, and sleep hygiene to enhance, accelerate, and ensure complete recovery.

Interaction of genes and environment shapes brain circuits (Rutten et al., 2013; Stewart-Knox et al., 2012; Presnell et al., 2007; Franklin et al., 2006). The process whereby there are changes in gene expression that do not involve changes to the underlying DNA sequence is called epigenetics. It is a complicated mechanism that occurs when a gene is modified by histone variants and methylation of DNA bases. These modifiers are the clothing that governs the pattern of gene expression and tells the gene to switch on or off. All this is to say that genes can change as a consequence of the environment in which they are expressed, and as such, dysfunctional behaviors are capable of being altered and reprogrammed. Our experiences govern the patterns of gene expression as to whether certain genes are turned on or off (Dupont, 2009).

Neurogenesis is the process during which lost nerve cells are replenished (Spalding et al., 2013; Dennis, Suh, Rodriguez, Kril, & Sutherland, 2016; Nyberg, 2012; Gorio, 1992). This is not a version of the familiar mitotic division of pre-existing neurons. Deep within the limbic system are the hippocampus and periventricular areas from which de novo cells are generated. Here lies a collection of stem cells or amplified progenitor cells that are stimulated by different thoughts and beliefs to proliferate and migrate to various cognitive areas of the neocortex to replace lost neurons. This replenishment contributes to the way we think, reason, and learn. It is logical to assume that these undifferentiated cells transform into active neurons that produce new memories, thoughts, emotions, feelings, and, subsequently, behaviors. Awareness that comes from being in the present through mindfulness practices, such as DBT, can ensure such neuroregeneration is accomplished both expediently and efficiently.

The brain functions not solely on the activation of specific locations within the brain, but involves multiple regions that form a complex integrated circuit throughout the brain. Often the brain evolves to be wired in a pattern that produces dysfunctional behaviors. Therapeutic interventions can improve on the original design through a process called neuroplasticity (Doidge, 2006; Arden, 2010; Jacobs, 2014; Raskin, 2011). This rewiring involves redirecting the connections (axonal guidance) and making new and stronger connections to other neurons (dendritic arborization). The strength of these new connections is dependent on synaptogenesis, which involves neurotransmitter concentrations and receptor sensitivity. The rewiring can be envisioned as a habit loop that manifests in new behaviors and is effectively orchestrated by CBT or behavior modification. Adjunctive interventions such as exercise and meditation can accelerate the process by adding growth factors such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF).

Resilience contributes to recovery in that it transforms the frightened victim into one capable of surviving during times of duress. This involves the process of adapting well in the face of adversity, whereby one bends but does not break. It is about flexibility and change when the old ways no longer work and invokes the ability to bounce back when one is faced with stress or pressure. Deliberate and consistent practice has been shown to reconfigure and increase the number and strength of connections in the resilience circuit (amygdala; medial, ventromedial, lateral, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; anterior cingulate cortex; and nucleus accumbens) (Southwick & Charney, 2012; Graham, 2013; Davidson & Begley, 2013; Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Amodio, Jost, Master, & Yee, 2007). Neurochemical changes include an elevation of dopamine and BDNFs with reduction in serotonin and glutamate in specific areas of the brain (Husseini et al., 2001).

It is entirely possible that brain locations designed for specific functions can transform into areas that serve a similar but alternative purpose. Take, for example, the location for sight that is found at the rear of the brain in an area called the occipital lobe. For a blind person, there is no activation when his or her eyes look at the pages of a book. However, while interpreting the writing by touch (Braille), brain imaging demonstrates that the occipital lobe is activated in the visual word-form area (Reich et al., 2011; Cohen, Scherzer, Viau, Voss, & Lepore, 2011; Bar-Cohen, 2009; Merabet et al., 2004). A change in function brought about by learning alternative processes in lieu of lost function would be an example of collateral repair.

This capacity to change is elucidated in the orchid hypothesis (Dobbs, 2012; Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007; Belsky & Pluess, 2009; Conley, Rauscher, & Siegal, 2013; Pluess et al., 2010; Ellis & Boyce, 2008; Aron, Aron, & Jagiellowicz, 2012). A dandelion is known to thrive in any environment. In contrast, the orchid wilts easily but grows magnificently in the proper environment. Applying this to humans, genes and traits may underlie our greatest weakness, but also underlie our strength and resilience if nurtured properly.

Often our thoughts and behaviors become locked, making change quite challenging. Stuck in our ways is analogous to a computer on which one is assigned to type a letter in Arial (font) 10 (point size). Initially, the type appears as Times Roman, 12 point, and one must manually pull down the appropriate font and size to continue the task at hand. No matter how many times we return to the computer to type a message, it will continue to first show up in the default mode. The only way to change this permanently is to go into the computer and adjust the hard drive. Antipsychotic drugs, deep-brain stimulation, and repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) have all demonstrated effectiveness in improving cognitive flexibility and are analogous to changing the brain’s hard drive (Pittenger et al., 2005, 2006, 2008, 2011; Ho et al., 2011; Bloch et al., 2006; Konradi & Heckers, 2001; Torres, 2013; Poeppl et al., 2014; Greenberg et al., 2008, 2010; Malone Jr. et al., 2009). One might imagine these interventions as applying WD-40 to the brain that enables therapy to be more effective in changing the way one thinks. Oxytocin reduces the likelihood of food obsessions, such as anxiously fixating on high-caloric foods, body shape, and negative emotions (Kim, 2014a, b, c). A dopamine agonist (olanzapine; Zyprexa®) might prove to be helpful for weight gain and anxiety reduction (Bissada, Tasca, Barber, & Bradwejn, 2008). Cognitive control over compulsive features of anorexia (i.e., feeling fat, rigid decision making, urges to restrict) might be reduced by magnetic pulses (rTMS) stimulating the dorsal left prefrontal cortex (McClelland et al., 2016a, b; Jay et al., 2016).

Anorectic patients highly value their cognitive function (intelligence) and can be motivated to pursue a legitimate course of treatment if they trust and believe in the process (Helgoe, 2008, 2011). Having insight into the neuroscience goes a long way in helping them understand their symptoms, accept treatment interventions, and utilize coping strategies. Those with anorexia can also find strength in those same traits that got them into trouble when they realize they can alternatively be a source of encouragement that can be projected as beneficial to their recovery. As deep and persistent thinkers, they possess analytical skills that integrate and process complex concepts and make them better at handling information overload (Cain, 2011, 2012). Though they take pride in their ability to lose weight, control their size, and manage their food intake; it is imperative that they connect with other accomplishments to redefine self and achieve personal fulfillment.

Critical to change is how the anorectic perceives the interventions to be effective. The psychoeducation that neuroscience brings forth has the potential to place them in their comfort zone. It is a nonthreatening way to address the disorder such that, by knowing the facts, they are better able to succeed. Presenting a neural map can construct a visual path of recovery and uncover biomarkers that predict outcomes. This helps the recovering anorectic feel more in control and in ownership of the disorder, which is necessary to stay with the program plan. Neuroscience allows the patients and treatment team to identify solutions for recovery and warnings of relapse (Guisinger, 2003; Kardum et al., 2008).

About the author:

Dr. Carson has been involved in the clinical treatment of obesity, addictions, and eating disorders for over 40 years. His unique background in health science and medicine (BS Duke University and B. H. S. Duke University Medical School) coupled with nutrition and exercise (BS Oakwood College, Ph.D. Auburn University) has prepared him to integrate neuropsychobiological intervention and proven psychotherapeutic treatment. Dr. Carson has honed his skills in communication and often-complicated science into enjoyable, practical and informative workshops. He is currently Vice President of Science and Innovation for the Eating Recovery Center’s & CORE Program for eating disorders treatment for people of higher weight in Denver, CO and consultant for the Pinegrove Behavioral Health and Addiction Center in Hattiesburg, MS. Ralph Carson, Ph.D. has consulted with numerous addiction and eating disorder treatment centers throughout the country, as well as being a highly sought after speaker at various conferences and workshops. Additionally, Dr. Carson has set up several eating disorder programs and corporate wellness programs. He is an active board member of the International Association of Eating Disorder Professionals (IAEDP). Working with Academy Medical Systems, he developed workshops for professional groups throughout the U. S. on topics such as exercise therapy, sports nutrition, eating disorders, and childhood obesity. He authored several popular books on nutrition, lifestyle practices, good health, and the brain: Harnessing the Healing Power of Fruits and the recently published The Brain Fix: What’s the Matter with Your Gray Matter?

[asa book]159979196X[/asa]

[asa book]0757316298[/asa]

References:

Ammar, A.A., Sederholm, F., Saito, T.R., Scheurink, A.J., Johnson, A.E., & Söderstein, P. (2000). NPY-leptin: Opposing effects on appetive and consummatory ingestive behavior and sexual behavior. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol, 278(6), R1627-R1633.

Amodio, D.M., Jost, J.T., Master, S.L., & Yee, C.M. (2007). Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience, 10(10), 1246-1247.

Arden, J.B. (2010). Rewire Your Brain: Think Your Way to a Better Life. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Aron, E.N., Aron, A., & Jagiellowicz, J. (2012). Sensory processing sensitivity: A review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Pers Soc Psychol Rev, 16(3), 262-282.

Baird, J.P., Gray, N.E., & Fischer, S.E. (2006). Effects of neuropeptide Y on feeding microstructure: Dissociation of appetitive and consummatory actions. Behav Neurocsi, 120(4), 937-951.

Bar-Cohen, Y. (2009). Dynamic Braille: The ability to read a full page of Braille text using refreshable displays would help people with visual impairments benefit from the growing advances in computer technology. SPIE Professional, October 2009. https://spie.org/membership/spie-professional-magazine/spie-professional-archives-and-special-content/oct2009-spie-professional/dynamic-braille

Bartness, T.J., Keen-Richard, E., Dailey, M.J., & Teubner, B.J. (2011). Neural and hormonal control of food hoarding. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol, 301(3), R641-R655.

Beaumont J. (Ed.). (1996). Australia’s War 1939-1945. St. Leonards, NSW: Allen & Unwin.

Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 300-304.

Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885-908.

Bissada, H., Tasca, G.A., Barber, A.M., & Bradwejn, J. (2008). Olanzapine in the treatment of low body weight and obsessive thinking in women with anorexia nervosa: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry, 165(10), 1281-1288.

Bloch, M.H., Landeros-Weisenberger, A., Kelmendi, B., Coric, V., Bracken, M.B., & Leckman, J.F. (2006). A systematic review: Antipsychotic augmentation with treatment refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder. Mol Psychiatry, 11(7), 622-632.

Botvinick, M.M., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Carter, C.S., & Cohen, J.D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652.

Bulik, C.M., Slof-Op’t Landt, M.C., van Furth, E.F., & Sullivan, P.F. (2007). The genetics of anorexia nervosa. Ann Rev Nutrition, 27, 263-275.

Burgess, E.E., Sylvester, M.D., Morse, K.E., Amthor, F.R., Mrug, S., Lokken, K.L., Osborn, M.K., Soleymani, T., & Boggiano, M.M. (2016). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on binge-eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord, 49(10), 930-936.

Cabanac, M., & Swiergiel, A.H. (1989). Rats eating and hoarding as a function of body weight and cost of foraging. American J Psychology, 257(4 Pt 2), R952-R957.

Cain, S. (2011). Are extroverts happier than introverts? Psychology Today, December 9, 2011.

Cain, S. (2012). Quiet: The Power of Introverts in a World That Can’t Stop Talking. New York: Crown Publishing Group.

Cloninger, C.R. (1986). A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatric Developments, 4(3), 167-226.

Cloninger, C.R., Svrakic, D.M., & Przybeck, T.R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 50(12), 975-990.

Cloninger, C.R., Przybeck, T.R., Svrakic, D.M., & Wetzel, R.D. (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to Its Development and Use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University.

Cohen, H., Scherzer, P., Viau, R., Voss, P., & Lepore, F. (2011). Working memory for Braille is shaped by experience. Commun Integr Biol, 4(2), 227-229.

Cohen, M.X., Young, J., Baek, J.M., Kessler, C., Ranganath, C. (2005). Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward response. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 851-861.

Conley, D., Rauscher, E., & Siegal, M.L. (2013). Beyond orchids and dandelions: Testing the 5-HTT “risky” allele for evidence of phenotypic capacitance and frequency-dependent selection. Biodemography and Social Biology, 59(1), 37-56.

Cozolino, L. (2010). The Neuroscience of Psychotherapy: Healing the Social Brain (Second Edition). New York: W.W. Norton & Co.

Cui, H., Moore, J., Ashimi, S.S., Mason, B.L., Drawbridge, J.N., Han, S., Hing, B., Matthews, A., McAdams, C.J., Darbro, B.W., Pieper, A.A., Waller, D.A., Xing, C., & Lutter, M. (2013). Eating disorder predisposition is associated with ESRRA and HDAC4 mutations. J Clin Invest, 123(11), 4076-4713.

Davidson, R.J., & Begley, S. (2013). The Emotional Life of Your Brain. New York: Plume.

Dennis, C.V., Suh, L.S., Rodriguez, M.L., Kril, J.J., & Sutherland, G.T. (2016). Human adult neurogenesis across the ages: An immunohistochemical study. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol, July 18, 2016.

Dharani, K. (2014). The Biology of Thought: A Neuronal Mechanism in the Generation of Thought – A New Molecular Model. Academic Press.

Dobbs, D. (2012). Can genes send you high or low? The orchid hypothesis a-bloom, March 20, 2012. http://www.wired.com/2012/03/can-genes-send-you-high-or-low-the-orchid-hypothesis-a-bloom/

Doidge, M. (2006). The Brain That Changes Itself. Penguin Group.

Ellis, B.J., & Boyce, W.T. (2008). Biological sensitivity to context. Curr Dir Psychol Sci, 17(3), 183-187.

Fassino, S. et al. (2002). Temperament and character profile of eating disorders: A controlled study with the Temperament and Character Inventory. Int J Eat Disord, 32, 412-425.

Franklin, J. et al. (2006). Obesity and risk of low self-esteem: A statewide survey of Australian children. Pediatrics, 118, 2481-2487.

Gorio, A. (1992). Neuroregeneration. Raven Press.

Graham, L. (2013). Bouncing Back: Rewiring Your Brain for Maximum Resilience and Well-Being. New World Library.

Greenberg, B.D. et al. (2008). The evolution of deep brain stimulation for neuropsychiatric disorders. Front Biosci, 13, 4638-4648.

Greenberg, B.D. et al. (2010). Deep brain stimulation of the ventral internal capsule/ventral striatum for obsessive-compulsive disorder: Worldwide experience. Mol Psychiatry, 15, 64-79.

Guisinger, S. (2003). Adapted to flee famine: Adding evolutionary perspective on anorexia nervosa. Psychol Rev, 110, 745-761.

Guisinger, S. Anorexia Nervosa: A Guide for Anorexics and their Loved Ones. http://www.drsarahravin.com/web/pdf/AN-Guisinger-article.pdf

Guisinger, S. (2012). Dangers of dieting: A body adapted to famine. F.E.A.S.T., March 2012. http://www.feast-ed.org/news/253590/Dangers-of-Dieting-a-Body-Adapted-to-Famine.htm

Halmi, K.A., & Sunday, S.R. (1991). Temporal patterns of hunger and fullness ratings and related cognitions in anorexia and bulimia. Appetite, 16, 219-237.

Halmi, K.A. et al. (2000). Perfection in anorexia nervosa: Variation by clinical subtype, obsessionality and pathological eating behavior. Am J Psychiatry, 157, 1799-1805.

Halmi, K.A. (2004). The neurobiology of eating disorders: A resurgence of investigation. CNS Spectr, 9, 510.

Helgoe, L. (2008). Introvert Power: Why Your Inner Life Is Your Hidden Strength. Naperville, IL: Sourcebooks (pp. 3-4).

Helgoe, L. (2011). Revenge of the introvert. Psychology Today, May 11, 2011.

Ho, B.C. et al. (2011). Long-term antipsychotic treatment and brain volumes: A longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 68, 128-137.

Hudson, J., Hiripi, E., Pope, H., & Kessler, R. (2007). The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Biol Psych, 61, 348-358.

Husseini, K. et al. (2001). The cellular neurobiology of depression. Nature Medicine, 541-547.

Jacobs, J. (2014). Neuroplasticity: Train Your Brain! Increase Cognitive Function, Improve Memory, and Get Smart Using Brain Plasticity. CreateSpace.

Jay, E.L. et al. (2016). Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of depersonalization disorder: A consecutive case series. Psychiatry Res, 240, 118-122.

Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The Long Shadow of Temperament. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Kagan, J. (2010). The Temperamental Thread: How Genes, Culture, Time and Luck Make Us Who We Are. Dana Press.

Kardum, I. et al. (2008). Evolution explanation of eating disorders. Psychology Topics, 17, 247-263.

Kaye, W.H. (1996). Neuropeptide abnormalities in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res, 62, 675-674.

Kaye, W.H. et al. (1998). Alterations in serotonin activity and psychiatric symptoms after recovery from bulimia nervosa. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 55, 927-935.

Kaye, W.H. et al. (1999a). The neurobiology of eating disorders. In Neurobiology of Mental Illness, D.S. Charney, E.J. Nestler, & B.S. Bunney (Eds.). New York: Oxford University Press (pp. 891-906).

Kaye, W.H. et al. (1999b). The neurobiology of eating disorders. In Neurobiology of Mental Illness, E.J. Nestler & D.S. Charney (Eds.). New York: Oxford University Press (pp. 1112-1128).

Kaye, W.H. et al. (1999c). Altered dopamine activity after recovery from restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, 503-506.

Kaye, W.H. et al. (2001). Altered serotonin 2A receptor activity in women who have recovered from bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry, 158, 1152-1155.

Kaye, W.H., & Walsh, B.T. (2002). Psychopharmacology of eating disorders. In Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress, Fifth Edition, K.L. Davis, C. Dennis, T. Coyle, & C. Nemeroff (Eds.). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins (pp. 1675-1683).

Kaye, W.H. et al. (2003). Anxiolytic effects of acute tryptophan depletion in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eating Disorders, 33, 257-267.

Kaye, W.H. et al. (2005a). Serotonin alterations in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: New insights from imaging studies. Physiol Behav, 85, 73-81.

Kaye, W.H. et al. (2005b). Brain imaging of serotonin after recovery from anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Physiol Behav, 86(1-2), 15-17.

Kaye, W. (2008). Neurobiology of anorexia and bulimia nervosa: Purdue Ingestive Behavior Research Center Symposium Influences on Eating and Body Weight over the Lifespan: Children and Adolescents. Physiol Behav, 94, 121-135.

Kaye, W.H., Fudge, J.L., & Paulus, M. (2009). New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 10, 573-584.

Kaye, W.H. (2011). Eating disorders: Hope despite mortal risk. Am J Psychiatry, 157, 1799-1805.

Kaye, W.H. (2013). Nothing tastes as good as skinny feels: The neurobiology of anorexia nervosa. Cell, 36, 110-120.

Kersting, K. (2004). An evolutionary explanation for anorexia? Modern anorexia may stem from an adaptation that helped ancient nomadic people find food, according to a recently proposed theory. APA Monitor, 35(4), 22.

Keys, A. et al. (1950). The Biology of Human Starvation. Minneapolis, MN: The University of Minnesota Press.

Kim, Y.R. et al. (2014a). Intranasal oxytocin attenuates attentional bias for eating and fat shape stimuli in patients with anorexia nervosa. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 133-142.

Kim, Y.R. et al. (2014b). Intranasal oxytocin lessens the attentional bias to adult negative faces: A double blind within subject. Experiment Psychiatry Investig, 11, 160-166.

Kim, Y.R. et al. (2014c). The impact of intranasal oxytocin on attention to social-emotional stimuli in patients with anorexia nervosa: A double blind within-subject cross-over experiment. PLoS One, 9, e90721.

Klump, K.L. et al. (2004). Personality characteristics of women before and after recovery from an eating disorder. Psychological Medicine, 34, 1407-1418.

Konradi, C., & Heckers, B. (2001). Antipsychotic drugs and neuroplasticity: Insights into the treatment and neurobiology of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 729-742.

Lask, B. et al. (2005). Functional neuroimaging in early-onset anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord, 37(Suppl), S49-S51.

Lask, B., & Frampton, I. (2011). Eating Disorders and the Brain. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons.

Lauer, C.J. et al. (1999). Neuropsychological assessments before and after treatment in patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Psychiatric Res, 33, 129-138.

LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.

LeDoux, J. (2001). The Synaptic Self. New York: Viking.

Liu, A. (1979). Solitaire. New York: Harper and Row.

Liu, A. (2007). Gaining: The Truth About Life After Eating Disorders. New York: Hatchet Book Group.

Lopez, O.L. et al. (2004). Classification of vascular dementia in the Cardiovascular Health Study cognition study. Neurobiology of Aging, 25(Suppl 1), S483.

Madra, M., & Zeltser, L.M. (2016). BDNF-Val66Met variant and adolescent stress interact to promote susceptibility to anorexic behavior in mice. Transl Psychiatry, 6, e778.

Malone Jr., D.A. et al. (2009). Deep brain stimulation of the ventral capsule/ventral striatum for treatment-resistant depression. Biol Psychiatry, 65, 267-275.

Marzola, E. et al. (2013). Nutritional rehabilitation in anorexia nervosa: Review of the literature and implications for treatment. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 290.

McClelland, J. et al. (2016a). A randomised controlled trial of neuronavigated repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in anorexia nervosa. PLoS One, 11(3) e0148606.

McClelland, J. et al. (2016b). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) treatment in enduring anorexia nervosa: A case series. Eur Eat Disord Rev, 24, 157-163.

Merabet, L. et al. (2004). Feeling by sight or seeing by touch? Neuron, 42, 173-179.

Nyberg, F. (2012). Neuropeptides in Neuroprotection and Neuroregeneration. CRC Press.

Pittenger, C. et al. (2005). Clinical treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatry, 2, 34-43.

Pittenger, C. et al. (2006). Glutamate-modulating drugs as novel pharmacotherapeutic agents in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. NeuroRx, 3, 69-81.

Pittenger, C., & Duman, R.S. (2008). Stress, depression, and neuroplasticity: A convergence of Mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 88-109.

Pittenger, C. et al. (2011). Glutamate abnormalities in obsessive compulsive disorder: Neurobiology, pathophysiology, and treatment. Pharmacol Ther, 132, 314-332.

Pluess, M. et al. (2010). 5-HTTLPR moderates effects of life events on neuroticism: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, 34, 1070-1074.

Poeppl, T.B. et al. (2014). Amygdalohippocampal neuroplastic changes following neuroleptic treatment with quetiapine in first-episode schizophrenia. Int J Neuropsychopharmacology, 17, 833-884.

Presnell, K. et al. (2007). Body dissatisfaction in adolescent females and males: Risk and resilience. The Prevention Researcher, 14, 3-6.

Raskin, S.A. (2011). Neuroplasticity and Rehabilitation. The Guilford Press.

Reich, L. et al. (2011). A ventral visual stream reading center independent of visual experience. Curr Biol, 21, 363-368.

Roberts, B.W., & Pomerantz, E.M. (2004). On traits, situations, and their integration: A developmental perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 402-416.

Roberts, M.E. et al. (2007). A systematic review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychol Med, 37, 1075-1084.

Roberts, M.E. et al. (2010). Exploring the neurocognitive signature of poor set-shifting in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. J Psychiatr Res, 44, 964-970.

Rutten, B.P. et al. (2013). Resilience in mental health: Linking psychological and neurobiological perspectives. Acta Psychiatr Scand, 128, 3-20.

Saxena, S., & Feursner, J.D. (2006). Toward a neurobiology of body dysmorphic disorders. Clinical Focus Primary Psychiatry, 13, 41-48.

Simpson, J.A., & Campbell, L. (Eds.). (2013). The Oxford Handbook of Close Relationships. New York: Oxford University Press.

Southwick, S.M., & Charney, D. (2012). Resilience: The Science of Mastering Life’s Greatest Challenges. Cambridge University Press.

Spalding, K.L. et al. (2013). Dynamics of hippocampal neurogenesis in adult humans. Cell, 153, 1219-1227.

Stewart-Knox, B. et al. (2012). Associations between obesity (BMI and waist circumference) and socio-demographic factors, physical activity, dietary habits, life events, resilience, mood, perceived stress and hopelessness in healthy older Europeans. BMC Public Health, 12, 424.

Tchanturia, K. et al. (2007). An investigation of decision making in anorexia nervosa using the Iowa Gambling Task and skin conductance measurements. J International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 635-641.

Torres, U.S. et al. (2013). Structural brain changes associated with antipsychotic treatment in schizophrenia as revealed by voxel-based morphometric MRI: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 342.

Tryon, W.W. (2014). Cognitive neuroscience and psychotherapy: Network principles for a unified theory. Elsevier SciTech Connect, July 22, 2014. http://scitechconnect.elsevier.com/cognitive-neuroscience-psychotherapy-network-principles-unified-theory/

Van der Zee, H.A. (1998). The Hunger Winter: Occupied Holland 1944-1945. Lincoln, NE, and London: University of Nebraska Press.

Zakzanis, K.K. et al. (2010). Quantitative evidence for distinct cognitive impairment in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Neuropsychol, 4, 89-106.

RELATED ARTICLES

Most Popular

Recent Comments

Linda Cerveny on Thank you
Carol steinberg on Thank you
Julia on My Peace Treaty
Susi on My Peace Treaty
Rosemary Mueller, MPH, RDN, LDN on Can You Try Too Hard to Eat Healthy?
Deborah Brenner-Liss, Ph.D., CEDS, iaedp approved supervisor on To Tell or Not to Tell, Therapists With a Personal History of Eating Disorders Part 2
Chris Beregi on Overworked Overeaters
Bonnie Adelson on Overworked Overeaters
Patricia R Gerrero on Overworked Overeaters
Linda Westen on Overworked Overeaters
Zonya R on Jay’s Journey
Dennise Beal on Jay’s Journey
Tamia M Carey on Jay’s Journey
Lissette Piloto on Jay’s Journey
Kim-NutritionPro Consulting on Feeding Our Families in Our Diet-Centered Culture
Nancy on Thank you
Darby Bolich on Lasagna for Lunch Interview